Anger Proneness Predicts Coronary Heart Disease Risk : Prospective Analysis From the Atherosclerosis Risk In Communities (ARIC) Study
Abstract
Background—Increased research attention is being paid to the negative impact of anger on coronary heart disease (CHD).
Methods and Results—This study examined prospectively the association between trait anger and the risk of combined CHD (acute myocardial infarction [MI]/fatal CHD, silent MI, or cardiac revascularization procedures) and of “hard” events (acute MI/fatal CHD). Participants were 12 986 black and white men and women enrolled in the Atherosclerosis Risk In Communities study. In the entire cohort, individuals with high trait anger, compared with their low anger counterparts, were at increased risk of CHD in both event categories. The multivariate-adjusted hazard ratio (HR) (95% CI) was 1.54 (95% CI 1.10 to 2.16) for combined CHD and 1.75 (95% CI 1.17 to 2.64) for “hard” events. Heterogeneity of effect was observed by hypertensive status. Among normotensive individuals, the risk of combined CHD and of “hard” events increased monotonically with increasing levels of trait anger. The multivariate-adjusted HR of CHD for high versus low anger was 2.20 (95% CI 1.36 to 3.55) and for moderate versus low anger was 1.32 (95% CI 0.94 to 1.84). For “hard” events, the multivariate-adjusted HRs were 2.69 (95% CI 1.48 to 4.90) and 1.35 (95% CI 0.87 to 2.10), respectively. No statistically significant association between trait anger and incident CHD risk was observed among hypertensive individuals.
Conclusions—Proneness to anger places normotensive middle-aged men and women at significant risk for CHD morbidity and death independent of the established biological risk factors.
The deleterious effects of anger on cardiovascular health is increasingly gaining attention in the research literature. Evidence for an anger–coronary heart disease (CHD) association has been derived from studies that use different measures of anger, different CHD end points, and different study designs.1 2 3 4 5 Trait anger, the focus of the present study, is a relatively stable personality trait that is manifested in the frequency, intensity, and duration of the anger experience.6 Persons with high compared with low trait anger have rage and fury more often, more intensely, and with longer-lasting episodes. Persons with high trait anger, by virtue of their propensity toward anger and their long-term exposure to its pathophysiological sequelae, might be particularly susceptible to CHD. However, this association may be attenuated in persons under treatment for hypertension because many pharmacological agents used to treat this condition may reduce the expression of CHD and the physiological responses to stress.
Studies have linked this personality attribute to CHD risk factors. Johnson et al,7 in cross-sectional analyses, showed that trait anger was positively associated with the LDL/HDL ratio and triglyceride levels. Durel et al8 reported a positive association between trait anger and blood pressure (systolic and diastolic) and between trait anger and cardiovascular reactivity. In a prospective analysis, Markovitz et al9 demonstrated that increases in trait anger predicted increases in blood pressure from baseline to 3 years of follow-up. Although these studies support a positive relation between trait anger and CHD risk factors, there are no published studies of the association between trait anger and incident CHD events.
The prospective association between anger (of any type) and CHD was described initially by investigators from the Framingham Heart Study, who reported that suppressed anger independently predicted the 8-year incidence of CHD among both men and women.10 More recently, Kawachi et al5 examined whether problems controlling one’s anger would predict CHD events among men in the Normative Aging Study. The investigators reported a 3-fold increase in CHD risk among individuals with the greatest difficulty controlling their anger as compared with those with the least. Additionally, they found a dose-response relation between the degree of difficulty with anger control and CHD risk and shorter CHD-free survival among participants with increasingly greater anger control problems.
The present study investigated, in a prospective design, the association of trait anger to CHD risk in a large bi-ethnic cohort of middle- to older-aged men and women enrolled in the Atherosclerosis Risk In Communities (ARIC) study.11 We assessed whether there was a dose-response relation between level of anger and CHD risk, whether the probability of CHD-free survival differed depending on the level of anger, and whether the trait anger–CHD association differed by hypertensive status.
Methods
Study Population
The study participants were white and black men and women who completed the Spielberger Trait Anger Scale (n=14 348) at the second examination of the ARIC study. ARIC is a population-based prospective study of cardiovascular disease designed with both cohort and surveillance components. The ARIC participants, aged 45 to 64 years at baseline, were recruited from random probability samples in the US communities of Washington County, Maryland; suburban Minneapolis, Minn; Forsyth County, North Carolina; and Jackson, Miss.11 Blacks were oversampled in Forsyth County, North Carolina, and exclusively sampled in Jackson, Miss. Since the baseline evaluations in 1987 to 1989, triennial clinical examinations and annual morbidity and mortality surveillance have been conducted.
Of the 14 348 individuals examined at the second visit (a return rate of 92.9% from baseline to the second clinic visit), 13 208 were free of clinically manifest CHD (defined as having no history of MI or cardiac revascularization procedures or ECG evidence of MI). Excluded from the analyses were 38 individuals with a racial/ethnic identity other than black or white; 40 with missing data on hypertensive status; and 144 with incomplete responses on the anger questionnaire. After these sequential exclusions, a total of 12 986 participants remained for the current analyses.
Assessment of Trait Anger
During the second clinical examination, participants completed the Health-Life Profile, which contained the 10-item Spielberger Trait Anger Scale.6 On this scale, respondents rated their typical experience with anger on a 4-point anchor: Almost Never=1, Sometimes=2, Often=3, and Almost Always=4. An overall anger score was obtained by summing each of the individual items. See the Appendix for the questionnaire items.
Assessment of CHD Risk Factors
All physiological measurements and laboratory assessments were made by trained technicians who used standardized protocols. Three sitting blood pressure measurements were taken after a 5-minute rest period with the use of a random-zero sphygmomanometer. The average of the second and third readings was used as the measure of blood pressure reported in this study. Hypertension was defined as diastolic blood pressure ≥90 mm Hg, systolic blood pressure ≥140 mm Hg, or use within the past 2 weeks of medication to lower blood pressure. HDL cholesterol was measured enzymatically after precipitation of LDL-containing proteins using dextran-magnesium. LDL cholesterol was calculated with the use of the Friedewald formula. Diabetes was defined as a fasting serum glucose level ≥140 mg/dL and/or a history of diabetes, insulin therapy, or oral hypoglycemic medication use. Waist-to-hip ratio was the ratio of waist girth, abdominal circumference measured at the umbilicus, to hip girth, measured at the maximal gluteal protrusion. Data on alcohol consumption, cigarette smoking, and level of educational attainment were obtained by self-report. Participants were asked to bring all medication (prescription and over-the-counter) taken within the 2 weeks immediately preceding the clinic visit. Each medication was coded by trained and certified interviewers with the use of a computerized medication classification system.
CHD Events
Incident CHD events reported in this study occurred in the follow-up period from the participant’s clinic visit in 1990 to 1992 through December 31, 1995, for a median of 53 months (maximum 72). An incident CHD event was defined as one of the following: acute MI/fatal CHD, silent MI, or cardiac revascularization procedure. Hospital CHD events were identified and validated through abstraction of death certificates and hospital discharge summaries. Data gathered on in-hospital events included presenting symptoms, ECG readings, cardiac enzyme levels, and other supportive diagnostic indicators. A maximum of three 12-lead ECG tracings were read by the University of Minnesota ECG Reading Center, with the use of the Minnesota code. The criteria for MI were based on a combination of chest pain, cardiac enzymes, ECG changes, and autopsy findings. Diagnoses were made with the use of a computerized algorithm and physician review system. Additional information was gathered on out-of-hospital deaths from the decedent’s next of kin and appropriate physician’s and/or the coroner’s or medical examiner’s report. The CHD diagnostic classification was assigned by 2 physicians on the ARIC morbidity and mortality classification committee; discrepancies in the diagnostic classification were adjudicated by a third panel member. A more detailed description of the quality control procedures used in the ascertainment of CHD events has been reported previously.12
Statistical Analyses
The overall anger score (a numeric score of 10 to 40) was coded as a 3-level categoric variable in which scores of 22 to 40 defined high trait anger, 15 to 21 moderate anger, and 10 to 14 low anger. The cut-points were comparable to those used in previously published work with the Spielberger scale.13 14 The main regression analyses were fit by the use of 3 different methods of handling missing values on the anger scale to determine the most appropriate strategy: (1) deleting all observations with missing values, (2) imputing a value of 1 for all missing values, and (3) imputing the average for a participant’s nonmissing responses. Because the results from all 3 methods were similar, the analyses were restricted to observations with complete trait anger protocols. The association between trait anger and incident CHD was determined with the use of Cox proportional hazards regression, in which anger was entered into the model as an indicator variable. Heterogeneity of effect was examined by each of 11 covariates: age, race/ethnicity, sex, educational level, HDL cholesterol level, LDL cholesterol level, hypertensive status, diabetes status, smoking status, and alcohol drinking status; only interaction with hypertensive status was statistically significant (χ2(2 df)=5.91, P=0.05). The regression analyses were stratified by hypertensive status to test the hypothesis that persons with hypertension would be less likely to demonstrate an association between trait anger and incident CHD. The models were fit separately for combined CHD and for “hard” events, adjusting first for age, and second for the additional covariates. To assess the presence of a linear trend in CHD risk, anger was entered into each regression equation as a continuous variable. Crude probabilities of CHD event-free survival were determined by the Kaplan-Meier product-limit method. When building the survival curves, deaths from all other causes were censored at the date of death.
Results
A cardiovascular disease risk factor profile for the cohort at the first follow-up examination (1990 to 1992) is presented in Table 1. The mean Spielberger trait anger score was 16.0 (SD 3.7; range 10 to 40); 7.7% of the cohort scored in the high trait anger range, 55.2% in the moderate range, and 37.1% in the low range. High scorers were slightly younger, more likely to be men, and to have less than a high school education compared with participants who were moderate or low scorers. Further, high scorers were more likely to be smokers and drinkers, with slightly lower HDL cholesterol levels and higher waist-to-hip ratios than moderate or low scorers. A total of 416 individuals had CHD events in the 72-month follow-up period (median 53). The absolute risk of incident CHD in the population as a whole was 7.4 events per 1000 person-years; the attributable risk for the high anger state was 4.0 events per 1000 person-years.
In the entire cohort, the age-adjusted hazard ratio (HR) of CHD (95% CI) was 1.73 (95% CI 1.25 to 2.40) for high versus low trait anger and 1.12 (95% CI 0.91 to 1.38) for moderate versus low anger. The corresponding multivariate-adjusted HRs were 1.54 (95% CI 1.10 to 2.16) and 1.10 (95% CI 0.88 to 1.36). Similarly, the age-adjusted HR for “hard” events was 1.75 (95% CI 1.17 to 2.64) for high versus low trait anger and 1.04 (95% CI 0.79 to 1.35) for moderate versus low anger. The corresponding multivariate-adjusted HRs were 1.63 (95% CI 1.07 to 2.48) and 1.08 (95% CI 0.82 to 1.43).
No racial/ethnic differences were found in the risk of incident CHD as the result of trait anger when this association was adjusted for the traditional CHD risk factors. Among whites, the age-adjusted relative hazard for combined CHD and for “hard” events was statistically significant, but when adjusted for the additional covariates, these results did not remain significant [1.51 (95% CI 0.88 to 2.59) and 1.50 (95% CI 1.01 to 2.23)]. Among blacks, neither the age- nor the multivariate-adjusted results were significant.
In contrast to the racial/ethnic comparisons, there were statistically significant differences in the hazard ratios for the association of trait anger and incident CHD among normotensive and hypertensive cohort members. Results of the proportional hazards regression models for both groups are presented in Table 2. Among normotensive individuals, the risk of combined CHD and of “hard” events increased monotonically as a function of trait anger. For combined CHD, the age-adjusted HR indicated that high anger scorers were 2.61 times more likely to have an event than low anger scorers. In addition, a 40% greater risk was observed among moderate compared with low anger scorers (probability value for linear trend 0.0006), although this HR was marginally significant. The relative risks were attenuated slightly after multivariate adjustment, but the patterns of risk and significance levels remained strong. The age-adjusted relative risk of high versus low anger for “hard” events was higher than that for combined CHD and similar for moderate versus low anger (probability value for linear trend 0.01). Again, the multivariate-adjusted point estimates for this subgroup were attenuated but remained significant (probability value for linear trend 0.02). These results indicate that the risk of “hard” events among high anger normotensive individuals, excluding those with only revascularization or silent myocardial infarction (MI), was nearly 3 times that of their low anger counterparts. Among hypertensive individuals, none of the HRs was statistically significant. In both CHD event categories, the pattern of risk for the age- and multivariate-adjusted models was similar.
The probability of CHD event-free survival was significantly lower among hypertensive than normotensive individuals (χ2(1 df)=71.6, P<0.0001). Among normotensives, the results of the log likelihood ratio tests indicated that CHD event-free survival differed significantly among high, moderate, and low anger scorers (χ2(2 df)=13.7, P=0.001) (Figure 1). The probability of CHD event-free survival was significantly lower among individuals reporting high trait anger compared with those reporting low anger (χ2(1 df)=15.2, P=0.0001) and compared with those reporting moderate anger (χ2(1 df)=8.0, P=0.005). Moderate anger scorers were marginally different from low scorers (χ2(1 df)=3.5, P=0.06). High, moderate, and low anger scorers among hypertensive individuals did not differ significantly in their probability of CHD event-free survival (χ2(2 df)=0.8, P=0.66) (Figure 2).
Because of heterogeneity of effect by hypertensive status, additional analyses were conducted to better understand the possible impact of medication use and blood pressure levels on the results obtained. As expected, χ2 analyses indicated that significantly more hypertensive than normotensive individuals used β-adrenergic antagonists (β-blockers), calcium channel blockers, ACE inhibitors, diuretics, and other antihypertensive medication (P=0.001). Hypertensive individuals were examined to determine whether medication use was related to trait anger status or to the incidence of CHD. High, moderate, and low anger scorers were significantly different from one another in their use of antianxiety (P<0.01) and antidepressant (P=0.033) medication but not in their use of antihypertensive agents (probability value range 0.16 to 0.78). A similar pattern was observed among normotensive individuals. Further, the association between medication use and the incidence of CHD among hypertensive individuals was statistically significant for calcium channel blockers [HR=1.84 (95% CI 1.36 to 2.50)] and ACE inhibitors [1.58 (95% CI 1.16 to 2.15)] but not for β-blockers [1.29 (95% CI 0.96 to 1.73], aspirin [0.88 (95% CI 0.65 to 1.19)], diuretics [0.97 (95% CI 0.74 to 1.26)], other antihypertensives [0.93 (95% CI 0.65 to 1.32), antianxiety agents [0.72 (95% CI 0.40 to 1.27)] or antidepressants [1.38 (95% CI 0.83 to 2.29)]. Entering medication use in the multivariate-adjusted regression model did not substantially affect the HRs [0.99 (95% CI 0.58 to 1.67) and 0.87 (95% CI 0.64 to 1.19)] for high and moderate trait anger, respectively. Medication data were complete for 87.9% of the study participants.
The anger-CHD relation was examined among participants who were not taking antihypertensive medication but in whom high blood pressure levels (systolic blood pressure ≥140 or diastolic blood pressure ≥90) were observed (n=1094). The multivariate-adjusted HR comparing high to low anger scorers was 0.65 (95% CI 0.22 to 1.93), indicating no significant association between CHD and trait anger. In contrast, a significant anger-CHD association was observed among normotensive individuals who were not taking antihypertensive medications [2.35 (95% CI 1.43 to 3.86)].
Discussion
In the entire cohort, high trait anger was associated with an increased risk of both combined CHD and of “hard” events. Among normotensive individuals, a high level of trait anger was associated with a slightly 2-times-greater risk of combined CHD and with a near 3-times-greater risk of “hard” events compared with a low level of anger. A dose-response relation was observed between anger and CHD in both event categories. CHD event-free survival was progressively shorter in low, moderate, and high anger groups. No significant association was found between trait anger and CHD risk among hypertensive individuals.
The precise biological mechanism and process by which anger influences CHD is yet to be clarified. One hypothesis is that the heightened sympathetic arousal and catecholamine secretion induced by anger are damaging to the heart and its vasculature and also play a role in the development of atherosclerotic lesions. Excessive circulating catecholamines (eg, epinephrine and norepinephrine) are known to cause direct damage to the endothelium and heart muscle15 16 and to disrupt the electrical rhythm of the heart.17 Further, studies have confirmed that the presence of catecholamines is associated with increased platelet adhesion and aggregation,18 lipid mobilization,19 and activation of macrophages,20 each of which has been implicated in the complex process of atherogenesis, stemming from endothelial injury and ending in the formation of atherosclerotic lesions.21
Evidence supports a direct relation between the rupture of atherosclerotic plaque, occlusive thrombosis, and the onset of acute MI.22 23 Increased attention is being paid to possible triggers of the sequence of events leading to MI because it has been demonstrated that these acute coronary syndromes do not occur randomly.24 25 Anger has been examined and found to be one such trigger.1 26 The relation is underestood from the framework of the triggering hypothesis in which anger can be viewed as a trigger of “acute risk factors” that in turn disrupt vulnerable atherosclerotic plaques.27 Acute risk factors are the physiological responses (eg, vasoconstriction, increased arterial pressure, increased platelet adhesion and aggregability, increased plasma coagulation, and fibrinolysis) that occur in close temporal proximity to an event and are responsible for the disruption of atherosclerotic plaque and the formation of an occlusive thrombus.27 When the disruption causes injury to the intimal and medial layers of the artery (type III lesion), occlusive thrombosis results and, within a brief period of time, acute MI or sudden death.27 28
The observed lack of association between anger and CHD risk among hypertensive individuals might have been due to the cardioprotective effects of medications that lower CHD morbidity and mortality rates. As expected, hypertensive individuals were found to use significantly more β-blockers, ACE inhibitors, calcium channel blockers, diuretics, and other antihypertensive agents than normotensive individuals. These medications, by preventing physiological responses such as vasoconstriction, thrombus formation, cardiac arrhythmias, and plaque disruption, might have forestalled a CHD event in hypertensive individuals who took them regularly. Two previous studies of the anger-CHD relation examined possible effect modification by the use of β-blockers and obtained contradictory results.1 5 One study found a lower risk of CHD among regular users and the second did not. Effect modification by aspirin use was also examined in those studies. In both, CHD risk was lower among aspirin users than among nonusers. Though we did not assess effect modification of CHD risk by aspirin use, the groups of hypertensive individuals and normotensive individuals in our study had similar proportions of individuals who took aspirin regularly.
We examined participants who were not taking antihypertensive medication but in whom blood pressure levels were high. Among them, we found no significant CHD risk associated with anger. Therefore, an alternative explanation for the lack of an observed anger-CHD association among hypertensive individuals may be that proneness to anger confers little or no additional CHD risk among individuals with high blood pressure, despite their medication use.
A strength of the present study is its prospective design. Because participants were free of clinically manifest CHD at baseline, the temporal ambiguity in the anger-CHD relation was virtually eliminated. Another strength of this study is that it is population-based, consisting of both black and white participants. To our knowledge, this is the first bi-ethnic, prospective study of the relation of anger to CHD, and in our study no racial/ethnic differences were observed. In addition, a relatively large number of events occurred in the observation period, and these events were validated according to standardized criteria. The classification of disease was based on data abstraction from medical records and discharge summaries, which, admittedly, is a process subject to error depending on the completeness and accuracy of data contained in the records. Another potential weakness of this study is the relatively marginal probability value of the hypertension interaction (P=0.05) in reference to the multiple stratifications that were conducted. However, this interaction is rather strong (despite the marginal probability value), as indicated by the nearly 3-fold increase in incident CHD risk in normotensives, while the association is entirely absent in individuals with hypertension.
In conclusion, these findings suggest that in addition to any adverse personal and social consequences that may accrue from having frequent and intense anger, there may be unfavorable cardiovascular-related outcomes as well. Anger proneness as a personality trait may place normotensive middle-aged men and women at significant risk for CHD morbidity and death independent of the established biological risk factors.
Appendix A1
Spielberger Trait Anger Scale
(1) I am quick tempered.
(2) I have a fiery temper.
(3) I am a hotheaded person.
(4) I get angry when I am slowed down by others’ mistakes.
(5) I feel annoyed when I am not given recognition for doing good work.
(6) I fly off the handle.
(7) When I get angry, I say nasty things.
(8) It makes me furious when I am criticized in front of others.
(9) When I get frustrated, I feel like hitting someone.
(10) I feel infuriated when I do a good job and get a poor evaluation.


Spielberger Trait Anger Scores | P 1 | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Low, 10–14 | Moderate, 15–21 | High, 22–40 | ||||
Participants, n (%) | 4821 (37.1) | 7165 (55.2) | 1000 (7.7) | |||
Age, y, mean (SD) | 57.3 (5.8) | 56.6 (5.7) | 56.3 (5.8) | <0.001 | ||
Male, n (%) | 2018 (41.9) | 3078 (43.0) | 464 (46.4) | 0.03 | ||
Less than high school education, n (%) | 1058 (22.0) | 1358 (19.0) | 292 (29.2) | <0.01 | ||
White, n (%) | 3481 (72.2) | 5545 (77.4) | 731 (73.1) | <0.01 | ||
Current smokers, n (%) | 866 (18.0) | 1706 (23.8) | 311 (31.1) | <0.01 | ||
Current drinkers, n (%) | 2500 (51.9) | 4288 (59.9) | 612 (61.2) | <0.01 | ||
Hypertensive, n (%) | 1711 (35.5) | 2434 (34.0) | 367 (36.7) | 0.09 | ||
Diabetic, n (%) | 548 (11.4) | 725 (10.2) | 136 (13.6) | <0.01 | ||
Plasma LDL cholesterol, mg/dL, mean (SD) | 133.1 (37.1) | 133.2 (36.5) | 132.1 (36.5) | 0.66 | ||
Plasma HDL cholesterol, mg/dL, mean (SD) | 50.4 (16.7) | 50.0 (16.8) | 49.0 (17.2) | 0.05 | ||
Waist-to-hip ratio, mean (SD) | 0.918 (0.083) | 0.925 (0.081) | 0.939 (0.081) | <0.001 |
1
One-way ANOVA test for comparison of means or χ test of association.
Spielberger Trait Anger Scores | |||||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Normotensives | Hypertensives | ||||||||||||||
Low, 10–14 | Moderate, 15–21 | High, 22–40 | P 1 | Low, 10–14 | Moderate, 15–21 | High, 22–40 | P 1 | ||||||||
Population, n | 3110 | 4731 | 633 | 1711 | 2434 | 367 | |||||||||
CHD events combined2 | |||||||||||||||
Individuals with incident events, n | 53 | 110 | 27 | 89 | 116 | 21 | |||||||||
Age adjusted | 1.00 | 1.40 (1.01–1.94) | 2.61 (1.64–4.16) | 0.0006 | 1.00 | 0.96 (0.73–1.27) | 1.14 (0.71–1.84) | 0.73 | |||||||
Multivariate adjusted3 | 1.00 | 1.32 (0.94–1.84) | 2.20 (1.36–3.55) | 0.02 | 1.00 | 0.95 (0.71–1.28) | 1.08 (0.67–1.76) | 0.73 | |||||||
“Hard” CHD events | |||||||||||||||
Individuals with incident events, n | 31 | 63 | 18 | 60 | 71 | 13 | |||||||||
Age adjusted | 1.00 | 1.36 (0.89–2.10) | 2.97 (1.66–5.31) | 0.005 | 1.00 | 0.88 (0.62–1.24) | 1.05 (0.58–1.92) | 0.85 | |||||||
Multivariate adjusted | 1.00 | 1.35 (0.87–2.10) | 2.69 (1.48–4.90) | 0.02 | 1.00 | 0.93 (0.65–1.34) | 1.00 (0.54–1.86) | 0.98 |
1
Probability value for linear trend.
2
Acute MI/fatal CHD, cardiac revascularization procedures, or silent MI.
3
Adjusted for age, race/ethnicity, level of educational attainment, sex, waist-to-hip ratio, drinking status (never, current, former), smoking status (never, current, former), plasma LDL and HDL cholesterol levels, and diabetes status.
Acknowledgments
This work was completed by the lead author while she was a postdoctoral fellow in the Department of Epidemiology at the University of North Carolina and supported by NIH, NHLBI, NRSA grant No. 5T32HL07055. The authors gratefully acknowledge individuals at the following ARIC field centers: University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill: Phyllis Johnson, Marilyn Knowles, Melisa LaVergne; University of Mississippi Medical Center, Jackson: Bobbie J. Alliston, Barbara L. Davis, Agnes L. Hayes; University of Minnesota, Minneapolis: Todd Avant, Joseph Bjorkland, Lori Bland; Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore: Pam Bowers, Joyce Chabot, Carol Christman; University of Texas Medical School, Houston: Nena Aleksic, Johnna Kincaid, Susan Mitterling; The Methodist Hospital, Atherosclerosis Clinical Laboratory, Houston, Tex: Charlie E. Rhodes, Julita Samoro, Val Creswell; Bowman-Gray School of Medicine, Ultrasound Reading Center, Winston-Salem, NC: Linda Allred, Carolyn Bell, Nancy Bourne; University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill, Coordinating Center: Jeffrey M. Abolafia, Nancy Y. Anderson, Hope E. Bryan.
References
1.
Mittleman MA, Maclure M, Sherwood JB, Mulry RP, Toffler GH, Jacobs SC, Friedman R, Benson H, Muller JE. Triggering of acute myocardial infarction onset by episodes of anger. Circulation.. 1995;92:1720–1725.
2.
Verrier RL, Hagestad EL, Lown B. Delayed myocardial ischemia induced by anger. Circulation. 1987;75:249–254.
3.
Boltwood MD, Taylor CB, Burke MB, Grogin H, Giacomini J. Anger report predicts coronary artery vasomotor response to mental stress in atherosclerotic segments. Am J Cardiol. 1993;72:1361–1365.
4.
Ironson G, Taylor CB, Boltwood M, Bartzokis T, Dennis C, Chesney M, Spitzer S, Segall GM. Effects of anger on left ventricular ejection fraction in coronary artery disease. Am J Cardiol. 1992;70:281–285.
5.
Kawachi I, Sparrow D, Spiro A III, Vokonas P, Weiss ST. A prospective study of anger and coronary heart disease: the Normative Aging Study. Circulation. 1996;94:2090–2095.
6.
Spielberger CD, Jacobs G, Russell S, Crane RS. Assessment of anger: the State-Trait Anger Scale. In: Butcher JN, Spielberger CD, eds. Advances in Personality Assessment. Vol 2. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1983:161–189.
7.
Johnson EH, Collier P, Nazzaro P, Gilbert DC. Psychological and physiological predictors of lipids in black males. J Behav Med. 1992;15:285–298.
8.
Durel LA, Carver CS, Spitzer SB, Llabre MM, Weintraub JK, Saab PG, Schneiderman N. Associations of blood pressure with self-report measures of anger and hostility among black and white men and women. Health Psychol. 1989;8:557–575.
9.
Markowitz JH, Matthews KA, Wing RR, Kuller LH, Meilahn EN. Psychological, biological and health behavior predictors of blood pressure changes in middle-aged women. J Hypertens. 1991;9:399–406.
10.
Haynes SG, Feinleib M, Kannel WB. The relationship of psychosocial factors to coronary heart disease in the Framingham study, III: 8-year incidence of coronary heart disease. Am J Epidemiol. 1980;111:37–58.
11.
ARIC Investigators. The atherosclerosis risk in communities (ARIC) study: design and objectives. Am J Epidemiol. 1989;129:687–702.
12.
White A, Folsom AR, Chambless LE, Sharret AR, Yang K, Conwill D, Higgins M, Williams OD, Tyroler HA, and ARIC Investigators. Community surveillance of coronary heart disease in the Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities (ARIC) study: methods and initial 2 years’ experience. J Clin Epidemiol. 1996;49:223–233.
13.
Denollet J, Brutsaert DL. Personality, disease severity, and the risk of long-term cardiac events in patients with a decreased ejection fraction after myocardial infarction. Circulation.. 1998;97:167–173.
14.
Lopez FG, Thurman CW. High-trait and low-trait angry college students: a comparison of family environments. J Couns Dev. 1993;71:524–527.
15.
Haft JI. Cardiovascular injury induced by sympathetic catecholamines. Prog Cardiovasc Dis.. 1974;17:73–86.
16.
Kones RJ. Emotional stress, plasma catecholamines, cardiac risk factors, and atherosclerosis. Angiology. 1979;30:327–336.
17.
Elhorras V, Zipes DP. Cardiac electrophysiologic alterations during myocardial ischemia. Am J Physiol. 1977;233:H329–H345.
18.
Haft JI. Catecholamines, stress, platelets and coronary artery disease. In: Mehta J, Mehta P, eds, Platelets and Prostaglandins in Cardiovascular Disease. New York, NY: Futura Publishing Co; 1981:265–277.
19.
Herd JA. Behavioral factors in the physiologic mechanisms of cardiovascular disease. In: Weiss SM, Herd JA, Fox BH, eds. Perspectives in Behavioral Medicine. New York, NY: Academic Press; 1981:55–66.
20.
Adams DO. Molecular biology of macrophage activation: a pathway whereby psychosocial factors can potentially affect health. Psychosom Med. 1994;56:316–327.
21.
Ross R, Glomset JA. The pathogenesis of atherosclerosis. N Engl J Med. 1976;295:420–425.
22.
Falk E. Plaque rupture with severe pre-existing stenosis precipitating coronary thrombosis: characteristics of coronary atherosclerotic plaques underlying fatal occlusive thrombi. Br Heart J.. 1983;50:127–134.
23.
Ridolfi RL, Hutchins GM. The relationship between coronary artery lesions and myocardial infarcts: ulceration of atherosclerotic plaques precipitating coronary thrombosis. Am Heart J.. 1977;93:468–486.
24.
Muller JE, Tofler GH, Stone PH. Circadian variation and triggers of onset of acute cardiovascular disease. Circulation. 1989;79:733–743.
25.
Tofler GH, Stone PH, Maclure M, Edelman E, Davis VG, Robertson T, Antman EM, Muller JE. Analysis of possible triggers of acute myocardial infarction (the MILIS Study). Am J Cardiol. 1990;66:22–27.
26.
Gabbay RH, Krantz DS, Kop WJ, Hedges SM, Klein J, Gottdiener JS, Rozanski A. Triggers of myocardial ischemia during daily life in patients with coronary artery disease: physical and mental activities, anger and smoking. J Am Coll Cardiol. 1996;27:585–592.
27.
Muller JE, Abela GS, Nesto RW, Tofler GH. Triggers, acute risk factors and vulnerable plaques: the lexicon of a new frontier. J Am Coll Cardiol. 1994;23:809–813.
28.
Fuster V, Badimon L, Badimon JJ, Chesebro JH. The pathogenesis of coronary artery disease and the acute coronary syndromes. N Engl J Med. 1992;326:242–250.
Information & Authors
Information
Published In
Copyright
Copyright © 2000 by American Heart Association.
History
Received: 18 August 1999
Revision received: 20 November 1999
Accepted: 1 December 1999
Published online: 2 May 2000
Published in print: 2 May 2000
Keywords
Authors
Metrics & Citations
Metrics
Citations
Download Citations
If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Select your manager software from the list below and click Download.
- Examining Cognitions in an Anger Episode with and Without Physical Aggression, Cognitive Therapy and Research, (2024).https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-024-10563-0
- Assessment and Management of Violent Behavior, Tasman’s Psychiatry, (5053-5082), (2024).https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-51366-5_3
- Kardiyovasküler Hastalıklar ve Mental Problemlerin İlişkisi, İstanbul Gelişim Üniversitesi Sağlık Bilimleri Dergisi, 23, (954-967), (2024).https://doi.org/10.38079/igusabder.1450641
- TANILAR ÖTESİ MODELİN AKIL YÜRÜTME HATALARI VURGUSUYLA PSİKOLOJİK PROBLEMLERE UYGULANMASI, Akademik Hassasiyetler, 11, 25, (331-364), (2024).https://doi.org/10.58884/akademik-hassasiyetler.1409958
- The Effects of Rational Emotive Behavior Therapy, Progressive Muscle Relaxation, Distraction, and Rumination on State Anger Using the Autobiographical Essay Memory Task, Journal of Aggression, Maltreatment & Trauma, 33, 10, (1240-1257), (2024).https://doi.org/10.1080/10926771.2024.2388561
- Patient and healthcare professionals' perceived barriers and facilitators to the implementation of psychosocial screening in cardiac practice: A Delphi study, General Hospital Psychiatry, 85, (104-113), (2023).https://doi.org/10.1016/j.genhosppsych.2023.10.002
- Validation of the Korean Adaptation of the State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory-2 (K-STAXI-2), STRESS, 31, 3, (133-141), (2023).https://doi.org/10.17547/kjsr.2023.31.3.133
- Implications of Social Disadvantage Score in Cardiovascular Outcomes and Risk Assessment: Findings From the Multi-Ethnic Study of Atherosclerosis, Circulation: Cardiovascular Quality and Outcomes, 16, 7, (e009304), (2023)./doi/10.1161/CIRCOUTCOMES.122.009304
- Assessment and Management of Violent Behavior, Tasman’s Psychiatry, (1-30), (2023).https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-42825-9_3-1
- The relationships between trait anger, forgiveness, and subjective well-being during the COVID-19 pandemic: a moderated mediating model under lockdown situation, Current Psychology, 42, 23, (20124-20133), (2023).https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-023-04500-9
- See more
Loading...
View Options
Login options
Check if you have access through your login credentials or your institution to get full access on this article.
Personal login Institutional LoginPurchase Options
Purchase this article to access the full text.
eLetters(0)
eLetters should relate to an article recently published in the journal and are not a forum for providing unpublished data. Comments are reviewed for appropriate use of tone and language. Comments are not peer-reviewed. Acceptable comments are posted to the journal website only. Comments are not published in an issue and are not indexed in PubMed. Comments should be no longer than 500 words and will only be posted online. References are limited to 10. Authors of the article cited in the comment will be invited to reply, as appropriate.
Comments and feedback on AHA/ASA Scientific Statements and Guidelines should be directed to the AHA/ASA Manuscript Oversight Committee via its Correspondence page.