Coronary Artery Calcium and Cognitive Function in Dutch Adults: Cross‐Sectional Results of the Population‐Based ImaLife Study
Abstract
Background
The aim of this study was to investigate whether increased severity of coronary artery calcium (CAC), an imaging biomarker of subclinical coronary atherosclerosis, is associated with worse cognitive function independent of cardiovascular risk factors in a large population‐based Dutch cohort with broad age range.
Methods and Results
A cross‐sectional analysis was performed in 4988 ImaLife participants (aged 45–91 years, 58.3% women) without history of cardiovascular disease. CAC scores were obtained using nonenhanced cardiac computed tomography scanning. The CogState Brief Battery was used to assess 4 cognitive domains: processing speed, attention, working memory, and visual learning based on detection task, identification task, 1‐back task, and 1‐card‐learning task, respectively. Differences in mean scores of each cognitive domain were compared among 4 CAC categories (0, 1–99, 100–399, ≥400) using analysis of covariates to adjust for classical cardiovascular risk factors. Age‐stratified analysis (45–54, 55–64, and ≥65 years) was performed to assess whether the association of CAC severity with cognitive function differed by age. Overall, higher CAC was associated with worse performance on 1‐back task after adjusting for classical cardiovascular risk factors, but CAC was not associated with the other cognitive tasks. Age‐stratified analyses revealed that the association of CAC severity with working memory persisted in participants aged 45 to 54 years, while in the elderly this association lost significance.
Conclusions
In this Dutch population of ≥45 years, increased CAC severity was associated with worse performance of working memory, independent of classical cardiovascular risk factors. The inverse relationship of CAC score categories with working memory was strongest in participants aged 45 to 54 years.
Nonstandard Abbreviations and Acronyms
- DET
- detection task
- IDN
- identification task
- OBK
- 1‐back task
- OCL
- 1‐card‐learning task
Clinical Perspective
What Is New?
•
In a Dutch population of individuals ≥45 years, increased coronary artery calcium severity was associated with worse performance of working memory, independent of classical cardiovascular risk factors.
•
The inverse relationship of coronary artery calcium score categories with working memory was strongest in individuals aged 45 to 54 years.
What Are the Clinical Implications?
•
Coronary artery calcium scoring may be useful to identify individuals at elevated risk of cognitive dysfunction and dementia.
The number of people with cognitive impairment and dementia is increasing worldwide because of growing older populations, and this has become a public health concern.1 Typically, there is a long preclinical stage before clinical symptoms of dementia become apparent.2, 3 This long time window offers the opportunity to investigate potential biomarkers that can identify individuals who are at risk of (early) cognitive changes, and subsequently, potential interventions can be applied in an early stage that may prevent or delay the cognitive decline. It has been shown that cardiovascular risk factors, such as smoking, hypercholesterolemia, hypertension, and obesity contribute to cognitive impairment and dementia.4 Coronary artery calcium (CAC), an imaging biomarker of atherosclerosis burden, reflects the effect of lifetime exposure to cardiovascular risk factors on the coronary arterial wall, and provides added value in predicting cardiovascular events.5 Thus, there may be a potential role for CAC to predict early cognitive decline and dementia. Prior studies have investigated the association between CAC and risk of dementia and cognitive decline, but results were inconsistent and with limited evidence supporting a significant inverse association.6, 7, 8, 9, 10, 11
Besides the aforementioned modifiable cardiovascular risk factors, chronological age is inversely associated with cognitive performance. Evident cognitive decline can start from the age of 45 years.12 The available evidence on associations between CAC and cognitive function at a relatively young age is limited.10, 13 However, knowledge about whether subclinical atherosclerosis is involved in early cognitive decline at a young age is important because this could offer the opportunity to intervene and reduce or halt atherosclerosis burden at middle‐life, and may prevent or slow down cognitive decline in later life.10, 14 Therefore, further research is needed to investigate the relation between subclinical atherosclerosis and cognitive function, not only in elderly but also in younger adults.
The ImaLife study, embedded in the Dutch population‐based Lifelines cohort, with comprehensive data collection of classical cardiovascular risk factors, nonenhanced cardiac computed tomography (CT) scans for CAC assessment, and validated cognitive testing based on a fast computerized tool, allows investigation of the association between CAC and cognitive function in a broad age group.15, 16 We examined the hypothesis that increased CAC severity is associated with worse cognitive function independently of classical cardiovascular risk factors. We further examined whether the association differed by age groups.
Methods
For access to the data that support the findings of this study, the Lifelines research office can be contacted via www.lifelines.nl/researcher.
Study Set‐Up and Population
The Lifelines study, a population‐based cohort study in the northern Netherlands, was launched in 2006 (baseline round) to investigate environmental, genetic, behavioral, physical, and psychological factors that may contribute to health and disease.15 In general, participants complete questionnaires every 1.5 years and revisit for examinations every 5 years. Data collection of the Lifelines cohort includes assessment of demographic and classical cardiovascular risk factors, as well as blood pressure measurements, blood laboratory tests, and cognitive function testing. The ImaLife study, embedded in the Lifelines cohort, was initiated in August 2017 to establish reference values of imaging biomarkers for early stages of coronary artery disease, lung cancer, and chronic obstructive pulmonary disease, as described in the study design article.16 All Lifelines subjects (aged ≥45 years) who were eligible for the ImaLife study were invited for a CT examination of heart and lungs. The ImaLife study was approved by the medical ethics committee of the University Medical Center Groningen, The Netherlands. Informed consent was given by all participants. For the current study, a shifted time cross‐sectional study design was used. We included 5162 participants who have completed cognitive function testing during the second round assessment (2014–2017) and had undergone cardiac CT scanning (2017–March 2020). In total, 174 participants were excluded; they either had a history of coronary heart disease (defined as self‐reported history of myocardial infarction, and/or coronary artery bypass grafting or percutaneous coronary intervention, and/or signs of myocardial infarction on electrocardiography) and/or a self‐reported history of stroke. A total of 4988 participants were included for the final analyses.
CAC Score
A low‐dose nonenhanced cardiac CT scan was performed using a third‐generation dual‐source CT (Somatom Force, Siemens Healthineers, Germany). The scan was performed with electrocardiographic triggering at 65% of the R‐R interval. The scan covered the whole heart from carina to the apex. Tube voltage was 120 kVp and reference of tube current was 64 mAs per rotation. Images were reconstructed with a field of view of 250 mm, slice thickness of 3.0 mm, and increment of 1.5 mm, with filtered back‐projection and medium‐sharp reconstruction kernel (Qr36d). Images were analyzed by well‐trained physician researchers (C.X. and R.M.) using commercial software (Syngo.via VB30A, CaScoring, Siemens). The Agatston score was used to quantify CAC.17 CAC scores were stratified into 4 common categories: 0, 1 to 99, 100 to 399, and ≥400 (Figure).

Figure 1. Example of participants with different CAC score in each risk category.
Participant with a CAC score of 0 (A), CAC score of 34 (B), CAC score of 200 (C), and CAC score of 768 (D). CAC indicates coronary artery calcium.
Assessment of Cognitive Function
At the second round assessment of Lifelines, the CogState Brief Battery, a collection of computerized tests, was used to measure multiple core cognitive domains: processing speed, attention, working memory, and visual learning. Compared with cognitive tests used in other studies on the relation between CAC and cognitive function, the CogState Brief Battery is a computerized test that is less time‐ and labor intensive, which makes it well suited to evaluate cognitive functioning in large cohorts. The CogState Brief Battery has good test–retest reliability and is a feasible tool to detect subtle cognitive changes in preclinical states of a general population as well as cognitive impairment in clinical patients.18, 19, 20 The CogState Brief Battery consisted of 4 tasks: (1) detection task (DET) measuring psychomotor function or speed of processing, (2) identification task (IDN) measuring visual attention, (3) 1‐back task (OBK) measuring working memory, and (4) 1‐card‐learning task (OCL) measuring visual learning.21 In the DET, participants were instructed by the on‐screen question “Has the card turned over?”. A face‐down playing card was presented in the center of the screen; participants must react to flipping over of the card by pressing the button “yes” as soon as possible. The task ends after 35 correct responses have been recorded. In the IDN, participants were instructed by the on‐screen question “Is the card red?”. A face‐down playing card was presented in the center of the screen. As soon as the playing card flips over, participants must decide whether the card is red or not by pressing the button “yes” or “no.” The task ends after 30 correct responses. In the OBK, participants were instructed by the on‐screen question “Is the previous card the same?”. Participants must decide whether the face‐up playing card is the same as the previous card or not by pressing the button “yes” or “no.” The task ends after 30 correct responses. In the OCL, participants were instructed by the on‐screen question “Have you seen this card before in this test?”. Participants must decide whether they have seen the face‐up playing card in this test or not by pressing the button “yes” or “no.” The task continued until 80 trials had been completed. The primary outcome of DET and IDN was the speed of the performance that was expressed as mean reaction time (ms, log10 transformed for normalization) for correct responses. The primary outcome of OBK and OCL was the accuracy of performance, which is the proportion of correct answers (arcsine square root transformed for normalization). Lower scores for DET and IDN and higher scores for OBK and 1‐card‐learning task indicated better cognitive performance. More detailed information on the CogState Brief Battery can be found online (www.cogstate.com).
Other Covariates
Detailed information on cardiovascular risk factors was based on questionnaires, physical examination, and blood laboratory tests in the Lifelines cohort at baseline and second round assessment. Use of medication was self‐reported, and recorded using anatomical therapeutic chemical codes at baseline. To define cardiovascular risk factors, data from the most recent assessment were used, supplemented with data from prior assessments in case of missing information.
Smoking habits and level of education were assessed by self‐report questionnaires. Smoking status was defined as current smoking or not by answering the question “do you smoke now or have you smoked in the past month?”. Educational level was categorized as low (≤12 years) or high (>12 years) according to the international standard classification of education.21, 22 Blood pressure measurements, laboratory blood tests, and anthropometric measurements were conducted following standardized protocols, as reported previously.15, 23 Hypertension was defined as self‐reported hypertension and/or systolic blood pressure ≥140 and/or diastolic blood pressure ≥90 mm Hg and/or use of antihypertensive medication. Hypercholesterolemia was defined as serum total cholesterol ≥6.2 mmol/L and/or use of lipid‐lowering medication. Diabetes mellitus was defined as self‐reported diabetes mellitus, and/or fasting glucose ≥7.0 mmol/L, and/or nonfasting glucose ≥11.1 mmol/L and/or glycated hemoglobin A1c ≥6.5% and/or use of oral antidiabetic medication or insulin. Body mass index was calculated as body weight divided by height squared (kg/m2).
Statistical Analysis
Population characteristics were described using frequency or percentage for categorical variables, mean and SD, or median and interquartile range for continuous variables depending on the distribution. Differences by sex were evaluated using independent t test or Mann–Whitney U test where appropriate. ANCOVA was used to calculate mean scores and 95% CI of the 4 cognitive tasks for each CAC category. Given that the effect of CAC categories on 4 cognitive outcomes was tested, a Bonferroni adjusted P value of <0.0125 (0.05/4) was considered statistically significant. Model 1 included adjustment for age; model 2 included additional adjustment for sex and educational level; and model 3 included additional adjustment for current smoking, hypertension, hypercholesterolemia, diabetes mellitus, and body mass index. These models were used to investigate whether the association between CAC and cognitive function was independent of cardiovascular risk factors. Tukey's post hoc tests were conducted to determine which CAC categories differed in scores of each cognitive task. All analyses were further stratified by 45 to 54, 55 to 64, and ≥65 years age categories to investigate whether there was a modification effect of age. Sensitivity analyses were performed by generating an alternative model adjusted for systolic blood pressure, total cholesterol, high‐density lipoprotein cholesterol, and low‐density lipoprotein cholesterol measured in the second round assessment (Table S1). A 2‐tailed P value of <0.05 was set as significance level, except for ANCOVA. R (Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria) was used for all analyses.
Results
Characteristics of the population are described in Table 1. Of the 4988 participants, 58.3% were female. Mean age was 57.4±8.4 years, ranging from 45 to 91 years. CAC was present in 47.9% of participants (63.8% of men and 36.6% of women). Median CAC score was higher in men (median 8, interquartile range 124) than in women (median 0, interquartile range 10, P<0.001). In participants with positive CAC, median CAC score was higher in men (median 62, interquartile range 262) than in women (median 33, interquartile range 108, P<0.001). Overall, the mean score of DET and IDN tests was 2.58±0.19 and 2.69±0.09, respectively. The mean score of OBK and OCL was 1.29±0.21 and 0.95±0.12, respectively. No differences of CogState subtasks were found by sex: DET (men 2.58±0.19, women 2.58±0.18, P=0.77), IDN (men 2.70±0.09, women 2.69±0.09, P=0.29), OBK (men 1.29±0.21, women 1.29±0.21, P=0.59), and OCL (men 0.95±0.12, women 0.95±0.12, P=0.34).
Characteristics | All |
---|---|
Age, y | 57.4±8.4 |
Age, % | |
45–54 y | 40.6 |
55–64 y | 39.3 |
≥65 y | 20.1 |
Sex, female % | 58.3 |
Race, White % | 98.8 |
Educational levels, high % | 30.4 |
Current smoking, yes % | 15.1 |
SBP, mm Hg, | 129.2±16.0 |
DBP, mm Hg | 75.2±9.6 |
Hypertension, yes % | 44.1 |
Total cholesterol, mmol/L | 5.25±0.95 |
HDL cholesterol, mmol/L | 1.59±0.43 |
LDL cholesterol, mmol/L | 3.43±0.44 |
Hypercholesterolemia, yes % | 25.7 |
Diabetes mellitus, yes % | 5.0 |
Body mass index, kg/m2 | 26.2±4.0 |
CAC score, AU % | |
0 | 52.1 |
1–99 | 30.6 |
100–399 | 10.6 |
≥400 | 6.7 |
John Wiley & Sons, Ltd
Values are number (percentage) or mean±SD. AU indicates Agatston units; CAC, coronary artery calcium; DBP, diastolic blood pressure; HDL, high‐density lipoprotein; LDL, low‐density lipoprotein; and SBP, systolic blood pressure.
Table 2 shows the mean scores of DET, IDN, OBK, and OCL according to CAC categories, for the different models. In the age‐adjusted analysis (model 1), significant differences in OBK scores were found across CAC categories, but not in DET, IDN, or OCL scores. This suggests that worse performances of working memory were associated with CAC severity, but psychomotor function, attention, and visual learning were not. Post hoc tests revealed significant differences between CAC ≥400 and CAC 0 for OBK scores. Adjustment for sex and educational level (model 2) did not substantially change overall differences in OBK. Further adjustment for cardiovascular risk factors (model 3) did not substantially change these results.
Cognitive tasks and Models | Coronary Artery Calcium Categories | P Value | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
0 | 1–99 | 100–399 | ≥400 | ||
Detection task | |||||
Model 1 | 2.58 (2.57, 2.59) | 2.58 (2.57, 2.59) | 2.57 (2.56, 2.59) | 2.58 (2.56, 2.60) | 0.867 |
Model 2 | 2.58 (2.57, 2.59) | 2.58 (2.57, 2.59) | 2.57 (2.56, 2.59) | 2.58 (2.56, 2.60) | 0.821 |
Model 3 | 2.58 (2.57, 2.59) | 2.58 (2.57, 2.59) | 2.57 (2.56, 2.59) | 2.58 (2.56, 2.60) | 0.816 |
Identification task | |||||
Model 1 | 2.69 (2.69, 2.70) | 2.70 (2.69, 2.70) | 2.70 (2.69, 2.71) | 2.69 (2.68, 2.70) | 0.195 |
Model 2 | 2.69 (2.69, 2.70) | 2.70 (2.69, 2.70) | 2.70 (2.69, 2.70) | 2.69 (2.68, 2.70) | 0.256 |
Model 3 | 2.69 (2.69, 2.70) | 2.70 (2.69, 2.70) | 2.70 (2.69, 2.70) | 2.69 (2.68, 2.70) | 0.261 |
1‐back task | |||||
Model 1 | 1.30 (1.29, 1.30) | 1.30 (1.29, 1.31) | 1.28 (1.26, 1.30) | 1.24 (1.22, 1.27)* | <0.001 |
Model 2 | 1.30 (1.29, 1.30) | 1.30 (1.29, 1.31) | 1.28 (1.26, 1.30) | 1.24 (1.22, 1.27)* | <0.001 |
Model 3 | 1.29 (1.29, 1.30) | 1.30 (1.29, 1.31) | 1.28 (1.26, 1.30) | 1.25 (1.22, 1.27)* | <0.001 |
1‐card‐learning task | |||||
Model 1 | 0.95 (0.95, 0.96) | 0.95 (0.94, 0.95) | 0.95 (0.93, 0.96) | 0.93 (0.91, 0.94) | 0.022 |
Model 2 | 0.95 (0.95, 0.96) | 0.95 (0.94, 0.95) | 0.95 (0.94, 0.96) | 0.93 (0.92, 0.94) | 0.063 |
Model 3 | 0.95 (0.94, 0.95) | 0.95 (0.94, 0.96) | 0.95 (0.94, 0.96) | 0.93 (0.92, 0.95) | 0.206 |
John Wiley & Sons, Ltd
Values are adjusted means and 95% CI. P value: overall difference between 4 coronary artery calcium categories. Model 1: adjusted for age. Model 2: adjusted for age, sex, educational level. Model 3: Model 2 plus adjusted for current smoking, body mass index, hypertension, hypercholesterolemia, and diabetes mellitus.
*
Post hoc tests, compared with coronary artery calcium 0, P<0.05.
Table 3 shows the results of age‐stratified analyses of associations among DET, IDN, OBK, and OCL scores and CAC categories. In participants aged 45 to 54 years, significant differences in OBK scores were observed across CAC categories, but not in DET, IDN, and OCL scores (model 1). Post hoc tests revealed that the OBK score in participants with CAC ≥400 was significantly lower than those with CAC 0, namely, 1.21 versus 1.33 (P=0.002). In multivariate models, the difference in OBK scores was slightly reduced but the relationship remained significant. Also in the older age categories, there was a trend towards lower OBK scores by CAC categories, but this did not reach statistical significance. In participants aged 55 to 64 years, there were small significant differences in IDN scores across CAC categories, but other scores did not differ significantly by CAC in multivariate models (model 3). Similarly, in participants aged ≥65 years, small differences in IDN scores across CAC categories were observed but not statistically significant.
Cognitive tasks and Models | Coronary Artery Calcium Categories | P Value | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
0 | 1–99 | 100–399 | ≥400 | ||
Aged 45–54 y, n=2027 | |||||
Detection task | |||||
Model 1 | 2.53 (2.52, 2.53) | 2.53 (2.51, 2.54) | 2.52 (2.49, 2.55) | 2.56 (2.51, 2.61) | 0.595 |
Model 2 | 2.53 (2.52, 2.53) | 2.52 (2.51, 2.54) | 2.52 (2.48, 2.55) | 2.55 (2.50, 2.60) | 0.646 |
Model 3 | 2.53 (2.52, 2.53) | 2.52 (2.51, 2.54) | 2.52 (2.48, 2.55) | 2.55 (2.50, 2.60) | 0.679 |
Identification task | |||||
Model 1 | 2.67 (2.66, 2.67) | 2.67 (2.66, 2.67) | 2.68 (2.66, 2.70) | 2.66 (2.64, 2.68) | 0.388 |
Model 2 | 2.67 (2.66, 2.67) | 2.67 (2.66, 2.67) | 2.68 (2.66, 2.69) | 2.66 (2.64, 2.68) | 0.489 |
Model 3 | 2.67 (2.66, 2.67) | 2.67 (2.66, 2.67) | 2.68 (2.66, 2.69) | 2.66 (2.63, 2.68) | 0.480 |
1‐back task | |||||
Model 1 | 1.33 (1.32, 1.34) | 1.34 (1.32, 1.35) | 1.29 (1.25, 1.33) | 1.21 (1.15, 1.27)* | <0.001 |
Model 2 | 1.32 (1.32, 1.34) | 1.34 (1.32, 1.35) | 1.29 (1.25, 1.33) | 1.22 (1.16, 1.28)* | <0.001 |
Model 3 | 1.32 (1.31, 1.33) | 1.34 (1.32, 1.36) | 1.29 (1.25, 1.34) | 1.23 (1.16, 1.29)* | 0.001 |
1‐card‐learning task | |||||
Model 1 | 0.96 (0.96, 0.97) | 0.96 (0.95, 0.97) | 0.96 (0.93, 0.98) | 0.93 (0.90, 0.97) | 0.486 |
Model 2 | 0.96 (0.96, 0.97) | 0.96 (0.95, 0.97) | 0.96 (0.93, 0.99) | 0.94 (0.90, 0.98) | 0.707 |
Model 3 | 0.96 (0.96, 0.97) | 0.97 (0.96, 0.98) | 0.96 (0.94, 0.99) | 0.95 (0.91, 0.98) | 0.691 |
Aged 55–64 y, n=1960 | |||||
Detection task | |||||
Model 1 | 2.59 (2.57, 2.60) | 2.59 (2.57, 2.60) | 2.57 (2.55, 2.60) | 2.57 (2.54, 2.61) | 0.651 |
Model 2 | 2.59 (2.58, 2.60) | 2.59 (2.57, 2.60) | 2.57 (2.55, 2.59) | 2.57 (2.53, 2.61) | 0.527 |
Model 3 | 2.59 (2.57, 2.60) | 2.59 (2.57, 2.60) | 2.57 (2.55, 2.59) | 2.57 (2.53, 2.61) | 0.623 |
Identification task | |||||
Model 1 | 2.69 (2.69, 2.70) | 2.70 (2.70, 2.71) | 2.68 (2.67, 2.70) | 2.68 (2.67, 2.70) | 0.013 |
Model 2 | 2.69 (2.69, 2.70) | 2.70 (2.70, 2.71) | 2.68 (2.67, 2.70) | 2.68 (2.67, 2.70) | 0.015 |
Model 3 | 2.69 (2.69, 2.70) | 2.70 (2.69, 2.71) | 2.68 (2.67, 2.69) | 2.68 (2.66, 2.70)† | 0.010 |
1‐back task | |||||
Model 1 | 1.29 (1.29, 1.30) | 1.29 (1.28, 1.31) | 1.28 (1.25, 1.31) | 1.24 (1.20, 1.28) | 0.100 |
Model 2 | 1.29 (1.28, 1.30) | 1.29 (1.28, 1.31) | 1.28 (1.25, 1.31) | 1.24 (1.20, 1.28) | 0.107 |
Model 3 | 1.29 (1.28, 1.30) | 1.29 (1.28, 1.31) | 1.28 (1.25, 1.31) | 1.25 (1.20, 1.29) | 0.201 |
1‐card‐learning task | |||||
Model 1 | 0.95 (0.95, 0.96) | 0.94 (0.94, 0.95) | 0.94 (0.93, 0.96) | 0.93 (0.90, 0.95) | 0.149 |
Model 2 | 0.95 (0.95, 0.96) | 0.95 (0.94, 0.95) | 0.94 (0.93, 0.96) | 0.93 (0.91, 0.95) | 0.305 |
Model 3 | 0.95 (0.94, 0.96) | 0.95 (0.94, 0.96) | 0.95 (0.93, 0.96) | 0.93 (0.91, 0.96) | 0.678 |
Aged ≥65 y, n=1001 | |||||
Detection task | |||||
Model 1 | 2.68 (2.65, 2.70) | 2.68 (2.66, 2.70) | 2.68 (2.65, 2.71) | 2.68 (2.65, 2.71) | 0.990 |
Model 2 | 2.67 (2.64, 2.70) | 2.68 (2.65, 2.70) | 2.68 (2.65, 2.71) | 2.69 (2.66, 2.72) | 0.826 |
Model 3 | 2.67 (2.64, 2.70) | 2.68 (2.65, 2.70) | 2.68 (2.65, 2.71) | 2.69 (2.66, 2.72) | 0.745 |
Identification task | |||||
Model 1 | 2.74 (2.73, 2.76) | 2.74 (2.73, 2.75) | 2.76 (2.75, 2.78) | 2.74 (2.73, 2.75) | 0.042 |
Model 2 | 2.74 (2.73, 2.76) | 2.74 (2.73, 2.75) | 2.76 (2.75, 2.78) | 2.74 (2.73, 2.76) | 0.050 |
Model 3 | 2.74 (2.73, 2.76) | 2.74 (2.73, 2.75) | 2.76 (2.75, 2.78) | 2.74 (2.73, 2.76) | 0.055 |
1‐back task | |||||
Model 1 | 1.24 (1.21, 1.27) | 1.24 (1.22, 1.27) | 1.23 (1.20, 1.27) | 1.20 (1.17, 1.24) | 0.321 |
Model 2 | 1.24 (1.21, 1.28) | 1.24 (1.22, 1.27) | 1.24 (1.20, 1.27) | 1.20 (1.17, 1.24) | 0.301 |
Model 3 | 1.24 (1.21, 1.28) | 1.24 (1.22, 1.27) | 1.24 (1.20, 1.27) | 1.20 (1.17, 1.24) | 0.303 |
1‐card learning task | |||||
Model 1 | 0.92 (0.90, 0.94) | 0.92 (0.91, 0.93) | 0.92 (0.91, 0.94) | 0.91 (0.89, 0.92) | 0.425 |
Model 2 | 0.92 (0.90, 0.94) | 0.92 (0.91, 0.93) | 0.93 (0.91, 0.94) | 0.91 (0.89, 0.92) | 0.475 |
Model 3 | 0.92 (0.90, 0.94) | 0.92 (0.91, 0.93) | 0.93 (0.91, 0.94) | 0.91 (0.89, 0.93) | 0.533 |
John Wiley & Sons, Ltd
Values are adjusted means and 95% CI. P value: overall difference between 4 coronary artery calcium categories. Model 1: adjusted for age. Model 2: adjusted for age, sex, educational level. Model 3: Model 2 plus current smoking, body mass index, hypertension, hypercholesterolemia, diabetes mellitus.
*
Post hoc tests, compared with coronary artery calcium 0, P<0.05.
†
Post hoc tests, compared with coronary artery calcium 1 to 99, P<0.05.
Sensitivity analyses by alternating modeling adjustments to systolic blood pressure, total cholesterol, high‐density lipoprotein cholesterol, and low‐density lipoprotein cholesterol showed similar results as adjustments for clinical determinations of disease phenotypes based on these conditions. Detailed information of sensitivity analysis can be found in Table S2.
Discussion
In this population‐based study of individuals aged ≥45 years without history of cardiovascular disease, cognitive performance of working memory showed an inverse trend with increasing CAC scores, independent of cardiovascular risk factors. This inverse relationship of CAC score categories with working memory was strongest in participants aged 45 to 54 years. In participants aged 45 to 54 years, the mean OBK score of CAC ≥400 was 0.09 points lower than CAC 0.
Prior studies that investigated associations between CAC and cognitive function showed mixed results. In general, higher CAC scores were associated with worse performance in processing speed, despite heterogeneities in study population and cognitive tests.7, 8, 9, 10 In the Rotterdam study, a prospective study among elderly (mean age 69.4±6.7 years), increased CAC was associated with accelerated decline in processing speed and higher risk of dementia.7 In cross‐sectional studies, an inverse association between CAC and cognitive function was also found. Particularly, in the CARDIA study among middle‐aged adults (43–55 years), a higher CAC category was associated with a worse performance of processing speed, sustained attention, and working memory.10 In the ELSA‐Brasil study (mean age 50.9±8.8 years, range 35–74 years), higher CAC scores were associated with worse performance of processing speed, executive function, and attention. In the AGES‐Reykjavik study among elderly (mean age 76.3±5.4 years), lower scores on processing speed and executive function were strongly related to higher CAC score, but the relation between CAC and memory was not significant in fully adjusted analyses.9 In our study, we did not find a relation between CAC severity and processing speed as assessed by the detection task. Besides heterogeneities in population, a potential explanation of this lack of association may be because of the different test we used. In prior studies, processing speed was measured by the digit symbol substitution test or the trail‐making test.7, 8, 9, 10 These tests are sensitive and validated tools and have been extensively used in neuropsychological assessment for decades.24, 25 Although significant correlations were found between DET with the trail‐making test and digit symbol test, the magnitude of the correlations was low, suggesting that DET may not be 1‐to‐1 mapped to the trail‐making test or the digit symbol test.18
In our study, working memory showed an inverse trend with increasing CAC scores, independent of cardiovascular risk factors. Moreover, this inverse relationship of CAC score categories with working memory was strongest in participants aged 45 to 54 years. This result was similar to the CARDIA study that was conducted in middle‐aged adults (43–55 years).10 We found that in participants aged 45 to 54 years, the OBK score in CAC ≥400 was 0.09 lower than in CAC 0 category. To put this into context, in the current study the OBK score was 0.004 lower for every 1‐year increase in age. This implies that the difference in OBK scores that was observed between CAC ≥400 and CAC 0 may correspond to a difference of 20 years.
Impairment in working memory task has been observed in many neurodegenerative diseases including Alzheimer disease and vascular dementia.26 In our study, the observed association between severe CAC and declined working memory may be explained by the concept that CAC is a marker that reflects the lifetime exposure to (un)known risk factors shared between cardiovascular disease and noncardiovascular disease including dementia.6, 27, 28 This exposure to risk factors may also have resulted in damage to vulnerable brain structures that are involved in memory function, but this needs to be further investigated. Another explanation for the association is that CAC may reflect generalized atherosclerosis including that in the coronary arteries and also in cerebral vessels. Atherosclerotic lesions in cerebral vessels may result in chronic cerebral hypoperfusion, which may consequently lead to cognitive impairment.29 On the other hand, in our study, the lack of significant association between CAC and cognitive function in participants aged ≥65 years may be because of the selective survival bias wherein participants with severe CAC who were likely to have worse cognitive performance may not have survived to participate in this study.
The main strengths of this study were as follows. First, this study comprises a large population‐based sample with a wide age range, which allows age‐stratified analyses of associations between CAC severity and cognitive function. Second, embedded in the Lifelines cohort, standardized protocols with quality control were used for the comprehensive data collection on potential confounders including educational level, lifestyle, and cardiovascular risk factors. Third, 4 core cognitive domains were assessed by the CogState Brief Battery. CogState Brief Battery, a collection of computerized tests with good test–retest reliability, which are less time‐consuming and labor‐intensive, has been developed to monitor cognitive changes in large population‐based studies with wide age ranges.18, 30 This study also had limitations. An important limitation was that there was an interval of 2 to 6 years between the assessment of cognitive function and CAC scoring. It is possible that less healthy participants, especially those who had worse cognitive performance, were not able to participate in the ImaLife study and therefore did not undergo CAC scoring. This may lead to an underestimation of the magnitude of the inverse association between CAC severity and cognitive function, especially in the elderly. Second, this study was a cross‐sectional analysis of associations between CAC severity and cognitive testing of different domains at a single time point. Therefore, it is not possible to assess whether increasing CAC severity is associated with accelerated cognitive decline over time or earlier onset of dementia. Third, although we excluded participants with self‐reported history of stroke, given lack of brain imaging in Lifelines, we were not able to investigate whether the association between increased CAC severity and worse cognitive performance is mediated by cerebral lesions. Further research of the mechanistic pathways between atherosclerotic burden and cognitive impairment is needed.
Conclusions
In conclusion, increasing CAC score was associated with worse performance of working memory in a large population‐based Dutch cohort aged ≥45 years. This association was independent of classical cardiovascular risk factors. The inverse relationship of CAC score categories with working memory was strongest in participants aged 45 to 54 years. These findings suggest that CAC scoring may potentially identify individuals at risk of cognitive dysfunction, although longitudinal studies are needed to confirm that CAC is an independent predictor of cognitive impairment and dementia.
Sources of Funding
The ImaLife study is supported by an institutional research grant from Siemens Healthineers and by the Ministry of Economic Affairs and Climate Policy by means of the Public‐Private Partnerships Allowance made available by the Top Sector Life Sciences & Health to stimulate public–private partnerships.
Acknowledgments
We would like to thank all ImaLife participants and technicians, and also the Lifelines personnel.
Footnotes
Supplementary Material for this article is available at Supplemental Material
For Sources of Funding and Disclosures, see page 8.
Supplemental Material
Tables S1–S2
- Download
- 224.23 KB
References
1.
Livingston G, Sommerlad A, Orgeta V, Costafreda SG, Huntley J, Ames D, Ballard C, Banerjee S, Burns A, Cohen‐Mansfield J, et al. Dementia prevention, intervention, and care. Lancet. 2017;390:2673–2734.
2.
Scheltens P, Blennow K, Breteler MM, de Strooper B, Frisoni GB, Salloway S, Van der Flier WM. Alzheimer's disease. Lancet. 2016;388:505–517.
3.
Launer LJ. The epidemiologic study of dementia: a life‐long quest? Neurobiol Aging. 2005;26:335–340.
4.
Qiu C, Fratiglioni L. A major role for cardiovascular burden in age‐related cognitive decline. Nat Rev Cardiol. 2015;12:267–277.
5.
Greenland P, Blaha MJ, Budoff MJ, Erbel R, Watson KE. Coronary calcium score and cardiovascular risk. J Am Coll Cardiol. 2018;72:434–447.
6.
Fujiyoshi A, Jacobs DR Jr, Fitzpatrick AL, Alonso A, Duprez DA, Sharrett AR, Seeman T, Blaha MJ, Luchsinger JA, Rapp SR. Coronary artery calcium and risk of dementia in MESA (Multi‐Ethnic Study of Atherosclerosis). Circ Cardiovasc Imaging. 2017;10:e005349.
7.
Bos D, Vernooij MW, de Bruijn RF, Koudstaal PJ, Hofman A, Franco OH, van der Lugt A, Ikram MA. Atherosclerotic calcification is related to a higher risk of dementia and cognitive decline. Alzheimers Dement. 2015;11:639–647.e1.
8.
Suemoto CK, Bittencourt MS, Santos IS, Bensenor IM, Lotufo PA. Coronary artery calcification and cognitive function: cross‐sectional results from the ELSA‐Brasil study. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2017;32:e188–e194.
9.
Vidal JS, Sigurdsson S, Jonsdottir MK, Eiriksdottir G, Thorgeirsson G, Kjartansson O, Garcia ME, van Buchem MA, Harris TB, Gudnason V, et al. Coronary artery calcium, brain function and structure: the AGES‐Reykjavik study. Stroke. 2010;41:891–897.
10.
Reis JP, Launer LJ, Terry JG, Loria CM, Zeki Al Hazzouri A, Sidney S, Yaffe K, Jacobs DR Jr, Whitlow CT, Zhu N, et al. Subclinical atherosclerotic calcification and cognitive functioning in middle‐aged adults: the CARDIA study. Atherosclerosis. 2013;231:72–77.
11.
Xia C, Vonder M, Sidorenkov G, Oudkerk M, de Groot JC, van der Harst P, de Bock GH, De Deyn PP, Vliegenthart R. The relationship of coronary artery calcium and clinical coronary artery disease with cognitive function: a systematic review and meta‐analysis. J Atheroscler Thromb. 2020;27:934–958. https://doi.org/10.5551/jat.52928
12.
Singh‐Manoux A, Kivimaki M, Glymour MM, Elbaz A, Berr C, Ebmeier KP, Ferrie JE, Dugravot A. Timing of onset of cognitive decline: results from Whitehall II prospective cohort study. BMJ. 2012;344:d7622. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.d7622
13.
Weimar C, Winkler A, Dlugaj M, Lehmann N, Hennig F, Bauer M, Kröger K, Kälsch H, Mahabadi A‐A, Dragano N, et al. Ankle‐brachial index but neither intima media thickness nor coronary artery calcification is associated with mild cognitive impairment. J Alzheimers Dis. 2015;47:433–442. https://doi.org/10.3233/JAD-150218
14.
Rossetti HC, Weiner M, Hynan LS, Cullum CM, Khera A, Lacritz LH. Subclinical atherosclerosis and subsequent cognitive function. Atherosclerosis. 2015;241:36–41. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.atherosclerosis.2015.04.813
15.
Scholtens S, Smidt N, Swertz MA, Bakker SJ, Dotinga A, Vonk JM, van Dijk F, van Zon SK, Wijmenga C, Wolffenbuttel BH, et al. Cohort Profile: LifeLines, a three‐generation cohort study and biobank. Int J Epidemiol. 2015;44:1172–1180. https://doi.org/10.1093/ije/dyu229
16.
Xia C, Rook M, Pelgrim GJ, Sidorenkov G, Wisselink HJ, van Bolhuis JN, van Ooijen PMA, Guo J, Oudkerk M, Groen H, et al. Early imaging biomarkers of lung cancer, COPD and coronary artery disease in the general population: rationale and design of the Imalife (Imaging in Lifelines) study. Eur J Epidemiol. 2020;35:75–86. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10654-019-00519-0
17.
Agatston AS, Janowitz WR, Hildner FJ, Zusmer NR, Viamonte M Jr, Detrano R. Quantification of coronary artery calcium using ultrafast computed tomography. J Am Coll Cardiol. 1990;15:827–832. https://doi.org/10.1016/0735-1097(90)90282-T
18.
Mielke MM, Machulda MM, Hagen CE, Edwards KK, Roberts RO, Pankratz VS, Knopman DS, Jack CR Jr, Petersen RC. Performance of the CogState computerized battery in the Mayo Clinic Study on Aging. Alzheimers Dement. 2015;11:1367–1376.
19.
Maruff P, Thomas E, Cysique L, Brew B, Collie A, Snyder P, Pietrzak RH. Validity of the CogState brief battery: relationship to standardized tests and sensitivity to cognitive impairment in mild traumatic brain injury, schizophrenia, and AIDS dementia complex. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2009;24:165–178. https://doi.org/10.1093/arclin/acp010
20.
Fredrickson J, Maruff P, Woodward M, Moore L, Fredrickson A, Sach J, Darby D. Evaluation of the usability of a brief computerized cognitive screening test in older people for epidemiological studies. Neuroepidemiology. 2010;34:65–75. https://doi.org/10.1159/000264823
21.
Kuiper JS, Oude Voshaar RC, Verhoeven FEA, Zuidema SU, Smidt N. Comparison of cognitive functioning as measured by the Ruff Figural Fluency Test and the CogState computerized battery within the LifeLines Cohort Study. BMC Psychol. 2017;5:15. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-017-0185-0
22.
Unesco M . International standard classification of education: ISCED 1997 (re‐edition). Paris, France: United Nations Educational, Scientific and Cultural Organization; 1997.
23.
van der Ende MY, Hartman MHT, Hagemeijer Y, Meems LMG, de Vries HS, Stolk RP, de Boer RA, Sijtsma A, van der Meer P, Rienstra M, et al. The LifeLines Cohort Study: prevalence and treatment of cardiovascular disease and risk factors. Int J Cardiol. 2017;228:495–500. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijcard.2016.11.061
24.
Jaeger J. Digit symbol substitution test: the case for sensitivity over specificity in neuropsychological testing. J Clin Psychopharmacol. 2018;38:513–519. https://doi.org/10.1097/JCP.0000000000000941
25.
Bowie CR, Harvey PD. Administration and interpretation of the trail making test. Nat Protoc. 2006;1:2277–2281. https://doi.org/10.1038/nprot.2006.390
26.
Budson AE, Price BH. Memory dysfunction. N Engl J Med. 2005;352:692–699. https://doi.org/10.1056/NEJMra041071
27.
Blaha MJ, Silverman MG, Budoff MJ. Is there a role for coronary artery calcium scoring for management of asymptomatic patients at risk for coronary artery disease?: Clinical risk scores are not sufficient to define primary prevention treatment strategies among asymptomatic patients. Circ Cardiovasc Imaging. 2014;7:390–397.
28.
Handy CE, Desai CS, Dardari ZA, Al‐Mallah MH, Miedema MD, Ouyang P, Budoff MJ, Blumenthal RS, Nasir K, Blaha MJ. The association of coronary artery calcium with noncardiovascular disease: the Multi‐Ethnic Study of Atherosclerosis. JACC Cardiovasc Imaging. 2016;9:568–576.
29.
Gorelick PB, Scuteri A, Black SE, DeCarli C, Greenberg SM, Iadecola C, Launer LJ, Laurent S, Lopez OL, Nyenhuis D, et al. Vascular contributions to cognitive impairment and dementia: a statement for healthcare professionals from the American Heart Association/American Stroke Association. Stroke. 2011;42:2672–2713. https://doi.org/10.1161/STR.0b013e3182299496
30.
Darby DG, Pietrzak RH, Fredrickson J, Woodward M, Moore L, Fredrickson A, Sach J, Maruff P. Intraindividual cognitive decline using a brief computerized cognitive screening test. Alzheimers Dement. 2012;8:95–104. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2010.12.009
Information & Authors
Information
Published In
Copyright
© 2021 The Authors. Published on behalf of the American Heart Association, Inc., by Wiley. This is an open access article under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution‐NonCommercial License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited and is not used for commercial purposes.
Versions
You are viewing the most recent version of this article.
History
Received: 26 June 2020
Accepted: 16 November 2020
Published online: 2 February 2021
Published in print: 16 February 2021
Keywords
Subjects
Notes
(J Am Heart Assoc.2021;10:e018172. https://doi.org/10.1161/JAHA.120.018172.)
Authors
Disclosures
The PhD project of Congying Xia is part of the ImaLife project, which is funded by an institutional research grant from Siemens Healthineers and by the Ministry of Economic Affairs and Climate Policy by means of the Public‐Private Partnerships Allowance made available by the Top Sector Life Sciences & Health to stimulate public–private partnerships. Matthijs Oudkerk is involved in the company iDNA B.V. Marleen Vonder is a consultant for iDNA B.V. The remaining authors have no disclosures to report.
Funding Information
Siemens Healthineers
Ministry of Economic Affairs and Climate Policy
Metrics & Citations
Metrics
Citations
Download Citations
If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Select your manager software from the list below and click Download.
- Genomic Characterisation of the Relationship and Causal Links Between Vascular Calcification, Alzheimer’s Disease, and Cognitive Traits, Biomedicines, 13, 3, (618), (2025).https://doi.org/10.3390/biomedicines13030618
- Metabolic Syndrome Status Changes and Cognitive Functioning: Insights from the Lifelines Cohort Study, The Journal of Prevention of Alzheimer's Disease, 11, 5, (1283-1290), (2024).https://doi.org/10.14283/jpad.2024.90
- Risk factors for cognitive dysfunction amongst patients with cardiovascular diseases, Frontiers in Public Health, 12, (2024).https://doi.org/10.3389/fpubh.2024.1385089
- Ct-defined emphysema prevalence in a Chinese and Dutch general population, European Journal of Radiology, 176, (111503), (2024).https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ejrad.2024.111503
- Association of Arterial Stiffness and Atherosclerotic Burden With Brain Structural Changes Among Japanese Men, Journal of the American Heart Association, 12, 11, (2023)./doi/10.1161/JAHA.122.028586
- A Powerful Paradigm for Cardiovascular Risk Stratification Using Multiclass, Multi-Label, and Ensemble-Based Machine Learning Paradigms: A Narrative Review, Diagnostics, 12, 3, (722), (2022).https://doi.org/10.3390/diagnostics12030722
- Coronary Artery Calcium: A Risk Factor for Brain Aging?, Circulation: Cardiovascular Imaging, 14, 7, (e013062), (2021)./doi/10.1161/CIRCIMAGING.121.013062
- Vitamin K2 Holds Promise for Alzheimer’s Prevention and Treatment, Nutrients, 13, 7, (2206), (2021).https://doi.org/10.3390/nu13072206
Loading...
View Options
Login options
Check if you have access through your login credentials or your institution to get full access on this article.
Personal login Institutional LoginPurchase Options
Purchase this article to access the full text.
eLetters(0)
eLetters should relate to an article recently published in the journal and are not a forum for providing unpublished data. Comments are reviewed for appropriate use of tone and language. Comments are not peer-reviewed. Acceptable comments are posted to the journal website only. Comments are not published in an issue and are not indexed in PubMed. Comments should be no longer than 500 words and will only be posted online. References are limited to 10. Authors of the article cited in the comment will be invited to reply, as appropriate.
Comments and feedback on AHA/ASA Scientific Statements and Guidelines should be directed to the AHA/ASA Manuscript Oversight Committee via its Correspondence page.